A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content. Sign in or start your free trial.

In This Article

  • Summary
  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Protocol
  • Results
  • Discussion
  • Disclosures
  • Acknowledgements
  • Materials
  • References
  • Reprints and Permissions

Summary

In real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (rtfMRI), brain activity is experimentally manipulated as an independent variable, and behavior is measured as a dependent variable. The protocol presented here focuses on the practical use of rtfMRI as a therapeutic tool for psychiatric disorders such as nicotine addiction.

Abstract

It has been more than a decade since the first functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)-based neurofeedback approach was successfully implemented. Since then, various studies have demonstrated that participants can learn to voluntarily control a circumscribed brain region. Consequently, real-time fMRI (rtfMRI) provided a novel opportunity to study modifications of behavior due to manipulation of brain activity. Hence, reports of rtfMRI applications to train self-regulation of brain activity and the concomitant modifications in behavioral and clinical conditions such as neurological and psychiatric disorders [e.g., schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive Disorder (OCD), stroke] have rapidly increased.

Neuroimaging studies in addiction research have shown that the anterior cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and insular cortex are activated during the presentation of drug-associated cues. Also, activity in both left and right insular cortices have been shown to be highly correlated with drug urges when participants are exposed to craving-eliciting cues. Hence, the bilateral insula is of particular importance in researching drug urges and addiction due to its role in the representation of bodily (interoceptive) states. This study explores the use of rtfMRI neurofeedback for the reduction in blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) activity in bilateral insular cortices of nicotine-addicted participants. The study also tests if there are neurofeedback training-associated modifications in the implicit attitudes of participants towards nicotine-craving cues and explicit-craving behavior.

Introduction

Neurofeedback is an operant conditioning procedure through which humans or animals can learn to modulate neural activity in one or more brain regions. Training typically leads to behavioral modifications1. In principle, brain signals from one or more circumscribed brain regions are transformed into sensory feedback (e.g., visual, auditory, or tactile feedback), which is provided to the participant for control of brain activity by operant conditioning or other forms of learning. In the reversal of the traditional neuroimaging paradigm, neurofeedback studies modulate brain activity as an independent variable and measure behavior as a dependent variable. Thus, neurofeedback provides a novel approach to investigating the involvement of brain regions in different cognitive functions and how hyper- or hypoactivation of those brain regions can lead to abnormal behavior.

Neurofeedback has been used with different neuroimaging modalities like functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG), and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). EEG- and fNIRS-based neurofeedback paradigms have the advantages of higher temporal resolution, affordability, and portability2,3. However, they are characterized by low spatial resolution and an inability to access deeper brain regions. In addition, EEG has the computational complexity of the inverse problem for determining a source of neural activations from surface EEG signals4. However, with recent developments in real-time fMRI (rtfMRI), it is possible to access hemodynamic signals from all parts of the brain, with good spatial resolution (e.g., 2 mm3) and a temporal resolution of 720 ms5. Thus, fMRI overcomes the above-mentioned limitations possessed by fNIRS and EEG techniques.

Addiction to nicotine is one of the major causes of death across the world due to a number of diseases associated with smoking6. Recognized factors leading to nicotine addiction are social, environmental, psychological7, and genetic susceptibility8. On a neurobiological level, studies have shown activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), ventral tegmental area (VTA), ventral striatum, amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex (PFC), and insular cortex during the presentation of drug-associated cues in contrast to neutral control cues9,10,11,12,13,14. Activity in both left and right insulas are highly correlated with smoking urges when smokers viewed drug-associated cues15,16. The insula plays an important role in eliciting craving behavior17,18,19,20,21, as it is responsible for perception of the bodily state. It has been reported that smokers with lesions in their insular cortices were more likely to quit smoking than smokers with brain damage not involving the insula18.

One of the biggest challenges in existing smoking cessation methods is the high relapse rate22. More than 80% of smokers relapse within the first few months after quitting smoking23. Exposure to cues previously associated with drug use is a major reason for the high relapse rate in nicotine addiction24. This mechanism is called the incubation effect. The current protocol is developed to target the incubation effect assessed by an affective priming task. Previous studies have demonstrated that abstaining smokers have negative implicit attitudes toward smoking-related cues25,26,27,28. In the typical affective priming task, emotional priming stimuli modify the processing of an affective target so that the reaction time and accuracy of responses are changed29. In other words, if the prime and target stimuli are of the same valence, the reaction time in response to the target stimuli will be faster, and vice versa.

In the current study, it is hypothesized that downregulation of the bilateral anterior insular cortex will reduce craving, and hence, the valence of craving-inducing cues will change from negative to neutral, as attentional and associative biasing will move away from smoking-related cues30. The implicit behavior task is an affective priming task originally adapted from Czyzewska and Graham31. Based on the aforementioned hypothesis, it is anticipated to observe a decrease in reaction time in response to a combination of prime (craving eliciting picture or its neutral counterpart picture) and target words with positive valence after downregulation block as compared to baseline block. The priming task (Figure 2B) consists of a prime (i.e., a craving eliciting picture or its neutral counterpart picture32) and target word with positive or negative valence. The prime picture is presented for 200 ms, followed by a target word presented for 1 second. Stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) is 250 ms. Participants are then instructed to judge the valence of the target word (positive or negative) and respond by pressing a button as quickly and accurately as possible.

The rtfMRI system (Figure 1) consists of the following subsystems: (1) participant, (2) signal acquisition, (3) online signal analysis, and (4) signal feedback. Signal acquisition is carried out with a 3.0T Siemens Trio whole-body scanner using an echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence33. Procedures such as image reconstruction, distortion correction, and averaging of the signal are performed on the scanner computer. Once the images are reconstructed and preprocessed, they are exported to the signal analysis subsystem. The signal analysis subsystem is implemented using the Turbo Brain Voyager (TBV)34. TBV retrieves the reconstructed images and performs data processing that includes 3D motion correction and real-time statistical analysis using the general linear model35. TBV allows the user to draw regions of interest (ROIs) over multiple voxels on the functional images and extract average BOLD values of the ROI after each repetition time (TR). The time series of the selected ROIs are then exported to the MATLAB script that calculates and presents feedback to the participant.

Visual feedback of brain activity is provided to participants in the form of a graphically animated thermometer, with its bars changing in proportion to the percent BOLD changes in the ROIs. Several studies have used intermittent feedback (feedback provided to a participant after a number of TRs of the EPI sequence) for training participants36,37. However, in the current study, it was anticipated that participants would have greater difficulty in downregulating the BOLD signal in the anterior insula with continuous feedback due to insula’s role in sensory integration and involvement in processing visual feedback information38. Hence, it was presumed that continuous feedback would result in a conflict between two processes in the insular cortex, one process that increases the signal due to external feedback, and another that decreases the signal due to neurofeedback training. Hence, in this study, we provide feedback only at the end of each downregulation block (delayed feedback). Participants are shown a text (e.g., Euro 0.87) as visual feedback (Figure 2A,C) that indicates the amount of money they earned (monetary reward). This reward is proportional to the percentage downregulation achieved in the regulation block.

RtfMRI is a novel neurotechnology that may be able to overcome problems in therapeutic approaches to addiction treatment and may provide more reliable and effective interventions for reducing relapse. The long-term objectives of the current study are three-fold: 1) to test whether nicotine addicts can learn to downregulate BOLD signals in the anterior insula during the presence of stimuli eliciting craving behavior; 2) to examine whether neurofeedback training leads to modifications in craving behavior; and 3) to explore whether changes in craving levels during neurofeedback training of downregulation of the insula persists after six months of training without any other intervention. This article provides a detailed description of the rtfMRI experimental protocol and its different components. Also presented are sample data from the study and a discussion of this method’s future challenges and potential in addition research. The protocol presented is designed to investigate whether fMRI-based neurofeedback training can be used to study reductions in brain activity in the insular cortex of cigarette smokers. In addition, the protocol is intended to study relationships between activation of the insular cortex and the craving behaviors of cigarette smokers.

Protocol

The Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Tübingen and Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile approved the following rtfMRI protocol.

1. Hardware set-up

  1. The hardware represented in Figure 1 is prepared only once to set up the real-time export of the MRI data.
  2. Preparation of the MRI room is the same as the traditional fMRI measurement. Prepare the MRI room before arrival of the participant.
  3. Attach the 32 or 20 channels head coil to the scanner.
  4. Connect the projector’s VGA/HDMI cable and MR compatible response device (button box) to the stimulus computer.
    NOTE: In most of the MR set-up, the response device delivers the TR-trigger to the stimulus computer. This trigger helps to synchronize stimulus presentation and data acquisition.

2. Participant preparation outside of the scanner

  1. Prepare the consent form and questionnaires that participants need to fill out before their arrival.
  2. Once the participant arrives, explain the experiment and fMRI technique. Also, instruct the participant on how to perform the task (e.g., during the experiment, participants must keep their eyes open and always look at the pictures on display screen, and they should try not to move their heads inside the scanner).
  3. Ask the participant to sign the consent form and fill in the questionnaires required to assess craving levels.
  4. Ask participants to fill out the following questionnaires: 1) VAS-C: Visual Analogue Scale, Craving39, 2) QSU-b: Questionnaire of Smoking Urges–Brief Version40, and 3) cigarettes per day.
  5. Ask the participant to exhale through the carbon monoxide (CO) measuring device.
    NOTE: CO measurement41 indicates whether or not the participant refrains from smoking at least 3 h before arrival for the neurofeedback session. CO is measured in ppm (parts per million). Low CO values indicate that the participant did not smoke for at least a couple of hours prior to arrival. Participants should be asked to refrain from smoking so that the craving cues elicit high craving during the experiment. Participants with high CO values will be cautioned prior to the future neurofeedback session to ensure that they refrain from smoking before the rtfMRI session. Participants who do not refrain from smoking twice should be excluded from the study.

3. Participant positioning

NOTE: The procedure of participant positioning on the scanner table is similar to the traditional fMRI experiment.

  1. Ask the participant to remove all metallic objects from his or her pockets before entering the MRI room.
  2. Ask the participant to insert ear plugs before he or she lies down on the scanner table in a supine position.
  3. Use pads to fix the head position of the participant inside the head coil. This step helps to reduce head movement during the measurement.
  4. Lock the upper part of the head coil and fix the mirror to the head coil.
  5. Give the response device to the participant and place the response device according to the comfort of the participant.
  6. Ask the participant to close his or her eyes and mark the reference head position of the participant in between the eyebrows using laser light.
  7. Move the scanner table to place the marked position to the center of the MRI bore.
  8. Confirm with the participant that he or she is in comfortable position and that he or she can see the visual stimuli projected on the screen behind the scanner using the mirror. Adjust the mirror, if required.

4. Data acquisition

  1. During the initial scans, ask the participant to close his or her eyes and try not to move the head.
  2. Start the measurement with a localizer pulse sequence. This sequence is typically used to determine the slice position of the subsequently run anatomical scan and functional (EPI sequence) scans.
  3. Select the field of view (FOV) for the anatomical scan with the following parameters: TR = 11.5 ms, TE = 5 ms, 176 slices without slice gap, FOV = 240 x 240mm2, matrix = 256 x 256, flip angle = 18°, providing 1 mm3 isotropic voxels. The FOV of the sequence will cover the whole head of the participant.
  4. The field of view for the functional (EPI sequence) scan is aligned to the anterior commissure/posterior commissure line (AC-PC line). Adjust the position of the slices to cover the target region of interest. The parameters of the sequence are the following: TR = 1.5, FOV = 192 mm, 25 slices, voxel = 3 mm x 3 mm x 3 mm, flip angle = 70°.

5. FMRI neurofeedback

  1. Alert the participant that the neurofeedback run is starting and repeat the instructions provided earlier (e.g., that during the baseline, a block is represented by a “+” sign).
    NOTE: The participant must observe the image on the display screen. On the other hand, during the regulation block, represented by an arrow in a downward direction, participants should try to detach themselves from craving urges using some cognitive strategies. The amount of money shown at the end of each regulation block represents their performance. A higher amount of money represents better performance.
  2. Perform the neurofeedback run in which the baseline and regulation blocks are alternating (30 s each; Figure 2).
  3. On the stimulus computer, run the stimulus code written presentation software and press the Enter button after Ready appears on the screen. Presentation code is now waiting for the trigger to start the neurofeedback run.
    NOTE: The code is in sync with the TR triggers coming from the scanner. Therefore, it is the first step in preparation of running the neurofeedback run.
  4. On the analysis computer, run the in-house MATLAB toolbox and turbo brain voyager (TBV).
  5. In the MATLAB toolbox, enter the information specific to the participant, such as patient ID and neurofeedback run number.
  6. Press Generate protocol files to prepare the protocol file using the information inserted in step 5.5.
    NOTE: The protocol file contains information related to the condition timing (e.g., the time at which TR a specific condition should start and which stimulus to be presented). It will be used by both the MATLAB toolbox and TBV.
  7. Press the button execute on the GUI of the MATLAB toolbox. The code is now waiting to receive data from the TBV.
  8. In the TBV, select the protocol file generated in step 5.6. In addition, select the .roi file generated from the previous neurofeedback sessions.
    NOTE: Selecting an ROI file is important, as it will avoid crashing of the TBV software during the initial (10 TRs) period of the neurofeedback run.
  9. Prepare the EPI sequence to be implemented in the MR host computer. Press the start button in the TBV.
  10. Redraw the target ROIs in the TBV software according to the anatomical landmarks. The shape and size of the ventricles in the brain are used as landmarks to select the anterior insula. In addition, the wavy shape of the insular cortex is used precisely select the voxels related to bilateral anterior insula.
  11. Draw an ROI on the primary motor area (M1) using the central sulcus as an anatomical landmark. The primary motor area acts as a reference ROI to remove the effect of global BOLD increases and BOLD fluctuations due to head movement.
  12. At the end of each neurofeedback session, ask the participant, “What cognitive strategy were you using during the regulation block?”. In addition, ask the participant about his or her comfort level and whether or not she or she wishes to continue with the experiment.
  13. After four neurofeedback runs, select the radio button Yes for the transfer run.
    NOTE: A transfer run is similar to the neurofeedback run. However, participants perform self-regulation in absence of feedback. This helps to determine whether the learned self-regulation is transferred to the situation in which the participant will not receive neurofeedback (e.g., outside the scanner).

6. Control group

  1. Instruct the participants in the control group in the same manner as those in the experimental group. However, provide Yolked feedback to the participants.
    NOTE: In yoked feedback, the average amount of reinforcement (money) in both the experimental and control groups remains the same. The only difference between the two groups is contingency of the feedback provided to participants. For participants in the control group, the total amount of reinforcement (money) is distributed among the randomly assigned 40% of the downregulation trials. However, in the other 60% of trials, participants receive negative feedback (zero Euros). Thus, the participants in the control group do not get contingent feedback.

7. Offline analysis

  1. Preprocessing of fMRI data
    1. Use a statistical parametric mapping (SPM) toolbox to preprocess the fMRI data (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/ext/).
    2. Convert the fMRI data in DICOM format to NIFTI format using format conversion utility of the SPM batch function.
    3. Remove the initial 10 scans from each neurofeedback run data to avoid gradient equilibration effects42.
    4. Perform the realignment process to remove motion artifacts and align all the volumes to the first volume of the session. In addition, perform time slice correction to compensate for the slice acquisition delay43.
    5. Conduct segmentation of the anatomical scan, coregistration of the EPI sequence data and the anatomical data, and normalization to map the subject space data to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard brain template44.
    6. Using our in-house MATLAB code, extract the BOLD signals from cube-shaped ROIs created around the MNI coordinates corresponding to the bilateral anterior insula and primary motor cortex.
  2. Calculation of percentage change in the BOLD signal
    1. Calculate the percentage change in the BOLD signal based on the average change in BOLD signal for each ROI during the regulation block compared to the previous baseline block. The equation for percentage change in the BOLD signal is the following:
      figure-protocol-10966
  3. Analysis of explicit smoking behavior of participants
    1. Import the participant responses to questionnaires (i.e., QSU-b, VAS-C, CO measure, and cigarettes per day) to MATLAB.
    2. Test normality of the data using a one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test from MATLAB.
    3. A one-sample t-test should be used to compare responses of different questionnaires for each participant, and a paired sample t-test to compare scores between the experimental and control groups.
  4. Analysis of the implicit attitude towards craving-eliciting cues
    1. Extract the reaction time (RT) of affective priming trials for each participant from the log files generated by presentation software.
    2. Remove the outliers based on the RT (i.e., do not include trials with a RT longer than the 2x the standard deviation of the mean RT of the participant).
    3. Test normality of the data using a one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test from MATLAB.
    4. Compare downregulation effects on the median reaction times of unique combinations of primes (craving-inducing and neutral images) and targets (positive and negative words) for each participant, and compare between the experimental and control groups using a paired wise t-test.

Results

Four patients were recruited based on their scores on the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND)45 questionnaire for medium-level nicotine dependence (FTND score >4) and the number of cigarettes smoked every day (>15). In addition, it was ensured that the participants did not have any tattoo or metallic implants as per MRI safety measures of the institution. Five rtfMRI sessions were performed for each participant, in which the first four sessions were conducted over 2 weeks (2 sessions pe...

Discussion

Results from four participants demonstrate the possibility for cigarette smokers to learn to downregulate activation in the bilateral anterior insula in the presence of a craving-eliciting cues. Changes in the implicit and explicit smoking behaviors after neurofeedback training in the sample participant may be related to learned downregulation, as the participant did not go through any other clinical or experimental interventions during the course of the experiment. Change in the participant’s implicit behavior may...

Disclosures

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by Comisión Nacional de Investigación Científica y Tecnológica de Chile (Conicyt) through Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico, Fondecyt Postdoctoral grant (no. 3100648) Fondecyt Regular(projects no. 1171313 and no. 1171320) and CONICYT PIA/Anillo de Investigación en Ciencia y Tecnología ACT172121.  

Materials

NameCompanyCatalog NumberComments
MATHSWORKMATLAB version 2014a
Presentation - Neurobehavioral SystemsPresentation version 18.0
Brain Innovation B.V.Turbo Brain Voyager Version 2.6 or 3.0

References

  1. Fernandez, T., et al. EEG and behavioral changes following neurofeedback treatment in learning disabled children. Clinical Electroencephalography. 34, 145-152 (2003).
  2. Scarapicchia, V., Brown, C., Mayo, C., Gawryluk, J. R. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy: Insights from Combined Recording Studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 11, 419 (2017).
  3. Hinault, T., Larcher, K., Zazubovits, N., Gotman, J., Dagher, A. Spatio-temporal patterns of cognitive control revealed with simultaneous electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping. , (2018).
  4. Grech, R., et al. Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysis. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation. 5, 25 (2008).
  5. Van Essen, D. C., et al. The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage. 62, 2222-2231 (2012).
  6. World Health Organization. . WHO Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic, 2017. , (2017).
  7. Ringlever, L., Otten, R., de Leeuw, R. N., Engels, R. C. Effects of parents' education and occupation on adolescent smoking and the mediating role of smoking-specific parenting and parent smoking. European Addiction Research. 17, 55-63 (2011).
  8. Malaiyandi, V., Sellers, E. M., Tyndale, R. F. Implications of CYP2A6 genetic variation for smoking behaviors and nicotine dependence. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics. 77, 145-158 (2005).
  9. Brody, A. L., et al. Neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure. Biological Psychiatry. 62, 642-651 (2007).
  10. Childress, A. R., et al. Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interventions in drug dependence. NIDA Research Monography. 137, 73-95 (1993).
  11. Claus, E. D., Kiehl, K. A., Hutchison, K. E. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 35, 1209-1219 (2011).
  12. Franklin, T. R., et al. Limbic activation to cigarette smoking cues independent of nicotine withdrawal: a perfusion fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 32, 2301-2309 (2007).
  13. Grusser, S. M., et al. Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology. 175, 296-302 (2004).
  14. Buhler, M., et al. Nicotine dependence is characterized by disordered reward processing in a network driving motivation. Biological Psychiatry. 67, 745-752 (2010).
  15. Bonson, K. R., et al. Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 26, 376-386 (2002).
  16. Brody, A. L., et al. Brain metabolic changes during cigarette craving. Archives of General Psychiatry. 59, 1162-1172 (2002).
  17. Contreras, M., Ceric, F., Torrealba, F. Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science. 318, 655-658 (2007).
  18. Naqvi, N. H., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, H., Bechara, A. Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science. 315, 531-534 (2007).
  19. Hollander, J. A., Lu, Q., Cameron, M. D., Kamenecka, T. M., Kenny, P. J. Insular hypocretin transmission regulates nicotine reward. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105, 19480-19485 (2008).
  20. Forget, B., Pushparaj, A., Le Foll, B. Granular insular cortex inactivation as a novel therapeutic strategy for nicotine addiction. Biological Psychiatry. 68, 265-271 (2010).
  21. Scott, D., Hiroi, N. Deconstructing craving: dissociable cortical control of cue reactivity in nicotine addiction. Biological Psychiatry. 69, 1052-1059 (2011).
  22. Buczkowski, K., Marcinowicz, L., Czachowski, S., Piszczek, E. Motivations toward smoking cessation, reasons for relapse, and modes of quitting: results from a qualitative study among former and current smokers. Patient Prefer Adherence. 8, 1353-1363 (2014).
  23. Hughes, J. R., Stead, L. F., Hartmann-Boyce, J., Cahill, K., Lancaster, T. Antidepressants for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. , CD000031 (2014).
  24. Bedi, G., et al. Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biological Psychiatry. 69, 708-711 (2011).
  25. Bassett, J. F., Dabbs, J. M. A portable version of the go/no-go association task (GNAT). Behavior Research Methods. 37, 506-512 (2005).
  26. Huijding, J., de Jong, P. J., Wiers, R. W., Verkooijen, K. Implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking in a smoking and a nonsmoking setting. Addictive Behaviors. 30, 949-961 (2005).
  27. Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., Chassin, L. Implicit and explicit attitudes towards cigarette smoking: The effect of context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 22, 13-39 (2003).
  28. Swanson, J. E., Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G. Using the Implicit Association Test to investigate attitude- behavior consistency for stigmatized behavior. Cognition and Emotion. 15, 207-230 (2001).
  29. Asgaard, G. L., Gilbert, D. G., Malpass, D., Sugai, C., Dillon, A. Nicotine primes attention to competing affective stimuli in the context of salient alternatives. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 18, 51-60 (2010).
  30. Gilbert, D. G., et al. Effects of nicotine on brain responses to emotional pictures. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 6, 985-996 (2004).
  31. Czyzewska, M., Graham, R. Implicit and explicit attitudes to high- and low-calorie food in females with different BMI status. Eating Behaviors. 9, 303-312 (2008).
  32. Gilbert, D. G., Rabinovich, N. E. . International smoking images series (with neutral counterparts). , (1999).
  33. Bandettini, P. A., Wong, E. C., Hinks, R. S., Tikofsky, R. S., Hyde, J. S. Time course EPI of human brain function during task activation. Magnetic Resonance Medicine. 25, 390-397 (1992).
  34. Goebel, R. BrainVoyager - past, present, future. NeuroImage. 62, 748-756 (2012).
  35. Poline, J. B., Worsley, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Frackowiak, R. S., Friston, K. J. Estimating smoothness in statistical parametric maps: variability of p values. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography. 19, 788-796 (1995).
  36. Johnson, K. A., et al. Intermittent "real-time" fMRI feedback is superior to continuous presentation for a motor imagery task: a pilot study. Journal of Neuroimaging. 22, 58-66 (2012).
  37. Yoo, S. S., Jolesz, F. A. Functional MRI for neurofeedback: feasibility study on a hand motor task. Neuroreport. 13, 1377-1381 (2002).
  38. Craig, A. D. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 10, 59-70 (2009).
  39. Cox, L. S., Tiffany, S. T., Christen, A. G. Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 3, 7-16 (2001).
  40. Wewers, M. E., Rachfal, C., Ahijevych, K. A psychometric evaluation of a visual analogue scale of craving for cigarettes. Western Journal of Nursing Research. 12, 672-681 (1990).
  41. Allen, D. R., Browse, N. L., Rutt, D. L., Butler, L., Fletcher, C. The effect of cigarette smoke, nicotine, and carbon monoxide on the permeability of the arterial wall. Journal of Vascular Surgery. 7, 139-152 (1988).
  42. Weiskopf, N., et al. Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging: methods and applications. Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 25, 989-1003 (2007).
  43. Sladky, R., et al. Slice-timing effects and their correction in functional MRI. NeuroImage. 58, 588-594 (2011).
  44. Mazziotta, J., et al. A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences. 356, 1293-1322 (2001).
  45. Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., Fagerstrom, K. O. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction. 86, 1119-1127 (1991).
  46. Myrick, H., et al. Differential brain activity in alcoholics and social drinkers to alcohol cues: relationship to craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 29, 393-402 (2004).
  47. Tapert, S. F., Brown, G. G., Baratta, M. V., Brown, S. A. fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addictive Behaviors. 29, 33-50 (2004).
  48. Kilts, C. D., Gross, R. E., Ely, T. D., Drexler, K. P. The neural correlates of cue-induced craving in cocaine-dependent women. American Journal of Psychiatry. 161, 233-241 (2004).
  49. Li, Q., et al. Assessing cue-induced brain response as a function of abstinence duration in heroin-dependent individuals: an event-related fMRI study. PloS One. 8, e62911 (2013).
  50. Frank, S., Kullmann, S., Veit, R. Food related processes in the insular cortex. Front Hum Neurosci. 7, 499 (2013).
  51. deCharms, R. C., et al. Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102, 18626-18631 (2005).
  52. Ruiz, S., Birbaumer, N., Sitaram, R. Abnormal Neural Connectivity in Schizophrenia and fMRI-Brain-Computer Interface as a Potential Therapeutic Approach. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 4, 17 (2013).
  53. Habes, I., et al. Pattern classification of valence in depression. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2, 675-683 (2013).
  54. Li, X., et al. Volitional reduction of anterior cingulate cortex activity produces decreased cue craving in smoking cessation: a preliminary real-time fMRI study. Addiction Biology. 18, 739-748 (2013).
  55. Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., O'Donnell, P. H. Evaluation of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changes in T.O.V.A. scores, behavioral ratings, and WISC-R performance. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation. 20, 83-99 (1995).
  56. Janssen, T. W., et al. A randomized controlled trial into the effects of neurofeedback, methylphenidate, and physical activity on EEG power spectra in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 57, 633-644 (2016).
  57. Mayer, K., Wyckoff, S. N., Fallgatter, A. J., Ehlis, A. C., Strehl, U. Neurofeedback as a nonpharmacological treatment for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 16, 174 (2015).
  58. Gevensleben, H., et al. Distinct EEG effects related to neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 74, 149-157 (2009).
  59. Ramos-Murguialday, A., et al. Brain-machine interface in chronic stroke rehabilitation: a controlled study. Annals of Neurology. 74, 100-108 (2013).
  60. Biasiucci, A., et al. Brain-actuated functional electrical stimulation elicits lasting arm motor recovery after stroke. Nature Communication. 9, 2421 (2018).
  61. Du, J., et al. Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor recovery and motor cortex excitability in patients with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. European Journal of Neurology. 23, 1666-1672 (2016).
  62. Ang, K. K., et al. Facilitating effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor imagery brain-computer interface with robotic feedback for stroke rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 96, S79-S87 (2015).
  63. Wolbrecht, E. T., Chan, V., Reinkensmeyer, D. J., Bobrow, J. E. Optimizing compliant, model-based robotic assistance to promote neurorehabilitation. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering. 16, 286-297 (2008).
  64. Sitaram, R., et al. Closed-loop brain training: the science of neurofeedback. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 18, 86-100 (2017).
  65. Simkin, D. R., Thatcher, R. W., Lubar, J. Quantitative EEG and neurofeedback in children and adolescents: anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, comorbid addiction and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and brain injury. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 23, 427-464 (2014).
  66. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., et al. Assessing interactions in the brain with exact low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Philosophical transactions: Series A, Mathematical, physical, and engineering sciences. , 3768-3784 (2011).
  67. Meir-Hasson, Y., Kinreich, S., Podlipsky, I., Hendler, T., Intrator, N. An EEG Finger-Print of fMRI deep regional activation. NeuroImage. 102, 128-141 (2014).

Reprints and Permissions

Request permission to reuse the text or figures of this JoVE article

Request Permission

Explore More Articles

Real time FMRINeurofeedbackInsular CortexNicotine AddictionSmokingCravingInteroceptive ProcessingCue induced CravingImplicit AttitudesExplicit Craving

This article has been published

Video Coming Soon

JoVE Logo

Privacy

Terms of Use

Policies

Research

Education

ABOUT JoVE

Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved